
(PDF) Correlates of meal skipping in young adults: A systematic review
- June 26, 2021
- by
- admin
For original article click here
ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review
Authors:
Background Meal skipping rates may be highest during young adulthood, a period of transition and development. Although these dietary behaviours may increase future risk of chronic disease, limited research has investigated correlates of meal skipping in young adults. MethodsA systematic literature search was conducted to identify studies that investigated correlates of meal skipping behaviours in young adults (aged 18–30 years). EBSCO host, MEDLINE Complete, Global Health, Scopus, EMBASE, Web of Science and Informit platforms were searched for eligible articles. Correlates were defined as any factor that was either associated with meal skipping or was self-reported by the participant to have an influence on meal skipping. Randomised controlled trials, prospective cohort studies, case-control studies, nested case-control studies, cross-sectional studies, and longitudinal studies were eligible for inclusion. ResultsThree-hundred and thirty-one articles were identified, 141 full-text articles assessed for eligibility, resulting in 35 included studies. Multiple methodological and reporting weaknesses were apparent in the reviewed studies with 28 of the 35 studies scoring a negative rating in the risk of bias assessment. Meal skipping (any meal), defined as the skipping of any meal throughout the day, was reported in 12 studies with prevalence ranging between 5 and 83%. The remaining 25 studies identified specific meals and their skipping rates, with breakfast the most frequently skipped meal 14–88% compared to lunch 8–57% and dinner 4–57%. Lack of time was consistently reported as an important correlate of meal skipping, compared with correlates such as cost and weight control, while sex was the most commonly reported associated correlate. Breakfast skipping was more common among men while lunch or dinner skipping being more common among women. Conclusions This review is the first to examine potential correlates of meal skipping in young adults. Future research would benefit from stronger design and reporting strategies, using a standardised approach for measuring and defining meal skipping.
Figures – uploaded byFelicity PendergastAuthor content
All figure content in this area was uploaded by Felicity Pendergast
Content may be subject to copyright.
Discover the world’s research
- 20+ million members
- 135+ million publications
- 700k+ research projects
R E V I E W Open Access
Correlates of meal skipping in young
adults: a systematic review
Felicity J. Pendergast
*
, Katherine M. Livingstone, Anthony Worsley and Sarah A. McNaughton
Abstract
Background: Meal skipping rates may be highest during young adulthood, a period of transition and development.
Although these dietary behaviours may increase future risk of chronic disease, limited research has investigated correlates
of meal skipping in young adults.
Methods: A systematic literature search was conducted to identify studies that investigated correlates of meal skipping
behaviours in young adults (aged 18–30 years). EBSCO host, MEDLINE Complete, Global Health, Scopus, EMBASE, Web of
Science and Informit platforms were searched for eligible articles. Correlates were defined as any factor that was either
associated with meal skipping or was self-reported by the participant to have an influence on meal skipping. Randomised
controlled trials, prospective cohort studies, case-control studies, nested case-control studies, cross-sectional studies, and
longitudinal studies were eligible for inclusion.
Results: Three-hundred and thirty-one articles were identified, 141 full-text articles assessed for eligibility, resulting in 35
included studies. Multiple methodological and reporting weaknesses were apparent in the reviewed studies with 28 of
the 35 studies scoring a negative rating in the risk of bias assessment. Meal skipping (any meal), defined as the skipping
of any meal throughout the day, was reported in 12 studies with prevalence ranging between 5 and 83%. The remaining
25 studies identified specific meals and their skipping rates, with breakfast the most frequently skipped meal 14–88%
compared to lunch 8–57% and dinner 4–57%. Lack of time was consistently reported as an important correlate of meal
skipping, compared with correlates such as cost and weight control, while sex was the most commonly reported
associated correlate. Breakfast skipping was more common among men while lunch or dinner skipping being more
common among women.
Conclusions: This review is the first to examine potential correlates of meal skipping in young adults. Future research
would benefit from stronger design and reporting strategies, using a standardised approach for measuring and defining
meal skipping.
Keywords: Meal skipping, Young adults, Correlates, Systematic review, Eating behaviour
Background
Young adulthood, is a unique developmental phase
defined as a period of multiple transitions and the devel-
opment of independence [1, 2]. During this time, indi-
viduals develop the skills needed to engage and practice
behaviours, such as healthy eating, that track into later
life [3, 4]. Research suggests that young adults engage in
poor eating behaviours, such as low fruit and vegetable
consumption [4, 5], high consumption of energy-dense
snack foods [6], and frequently fail to consume regular
meals [7, 8].
Meal skipping is the omission or lack of consumption
of one or more of the traditional main meals (breakfast,
lunch or dinner) throughout the day [9]. The regular
omission of meals, particularly the breakfast meal, has
been associated with poorer diet quality [10], lower
intakes of total energy, vitamins and minerals [11–13],
increased risk of central adiposity [14, 15], markers of
insulin resistance [15, 16] and cardio metabolic risk fac-
tors [15, 17]. Estimated prevalence rates of meal skip-
ping in the young adult population vary between 24 and
87% [18, 19], with young adults consistently reporting
* Correspondence: fpenderg@deakin.edu.au
Institute for Physical Activity and Nutrition (IPAN), School of Exercise and
Nutrition Sciences, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Highway, Burwood,
Geelong, Victoria 3125, Australia
© The Author(s). 2016 Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0
International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to
the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver
(http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Pendergast et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition
and Physical Activity (2016) 13:125
DOI 10.1186/s12966-016-0451-1
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
higher rates of meal skipping compared with other age
groups [5]. Recent data from the Australian Health
Survey 2011/12 showed that 39% of Australian young
adults (19–24 years) reported eating breakfast less than
5 days per week, compared with 10% children (8–11
years) and 33% of all adults (>18 years) [5]. Despite
the significant health implications of meal skipping
and its higher prevalence among young adults, limited
research has investigated correlates of this unhealthy
eating behaviour.
Conceptual models or frameworks are useful in under-
standing and explaining correlates of eating behaviours
such as meal skipping. While there are a number of pos-
sible conceptual frameworks in the literature, this review
will use the framework developed by Story et al. [20] to
categorise the correlates of meal skipping. This frame-
work combines ecological perspectives with social cogni-
tive theories (SCT) resulting in a framework which takes
into account the relationship between people and their
environments seen in ecological models [21], and socio-
environmental, personal and behavioural factors seen
within SCT [22]. This social-ecological framework (SEF)
is made up of the following four domains: 1) Individual
influences (intrapersonal); 2) Social environmental influ-
ences (interpersonal); 3) Physical environmental influ-
ences (community settings); 4) Macrosystem influences
(societal) [20], and has been used effectively in eating be-
haviour research [20, 23]. Previous research investigating
correlates of meal skipping in various populations have
identified correlates from each of these domains. Individual
influences such as smoking status [24] and infrequent phys-
ical activity [25]; social environmental influences of family
support [24]; and physical environmental influences such as
housing type have been associated with meal skipping
behaviours [26].
Given the importance of this life stage in the devel-
opment of long term health behaviours and the high
prevalence of poor eating behaviours in this popula-
tion group, understanding the correlates of meal
skipping is needed to better inform public health
strategies and dietary interventions. However, there
have been no systematic reviews to synthesise the evi-
dence on correlates of meal skipping. Therefore, the
aim of the present review was to systematically evalu-
ate the literature on correlates of meal skipping in
young adults (18–30 years) using a SEF.
Methods
Protocol
This study followed the procedures for systematic review
reporting as described by the Preferred Reporting Items
for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA)
(Additional file 1) recommendations [27].
Search strategy
A systematic and comprehensive search of the literature
surrounding meal skipping was conducted in January
2016. The search was limited to human studies, pub-
lished in English post 1979. This time frame was chosen,
as the first studies describing eating patterns were pub-
lished in early 1980’s [28, 29]. Academic Search
complete, CINAHL Complete, PsycINFO, SocINDEX,
ERIC and Education Source were searched through
EBSCO Host. MEDLINE Complete, Global Health,
Scopus, EMBASE, Web of Science and Informit searches
were conducted independently. Bibliographies of in-
cluded articles were also reviewed (hand searched) for
additional articles. Search terms were tested prior to the
recorded search to ensure that appropriate articles were
identified. The following search terms were used during
the systematic searching of databases: (Meal skipping,
meal frequency, meal omission (skip* OR frequen* OR
omission*) N5 meal*) AND (Young adults, emerging
adult, college students (young OR emerge*) N5 adult*
(college OR university* OR undergraduate* OR “post-
secondary*”OR postgraduate*) N5 student*)) AND
(Eating habits, feeding habits, food habits, diet habits,
meal habits (diet* OR eat* OR meal* OR feed* OR
food*) N5 habit*).
Eligibility criteria
To be included in this review each article was required
to meet the following criteria: (1) original research art-
icle, published in a peer-reviewed journal, with full text
in English language; (2) the study participants were
young adults aged 18–30 years or with a mean age be-
tween 18 and 30 years, or aged 18–30 years at baseline
for longitudinal studies, for studies that did not report a
mean age, the participants needed to be referred to as
university or college students; (3) the study participants
were free from disease and were community-dwelling;
(4) there was a measure of meal skipping, meal omis-
sion, or meal frequency reported (assessed as any meal
skipped throughout the day or according to meal type
e.g. breakfast, lunch, dinner, and supper); (5) there was
at least one meal skipping correlate reported; (6) the
study design was one of the following: randomised con-
trolled trial, prospective cohort study, case-control
study, nested case-control study, cross-sectional study,
longitudinal study.
Articles were excluded if they met any of the following
criteria: (1) studies published as abstracts, conference
proceedings, posters or not in the English language; (2)
the article included specific populations of young adults
(e.g. athletes, institutionalised populations); (3) the par-
ticipants’mean age was outside the range of 18–30 years
or included children or older adults; (4) there was no
measure or report of meal skipping, meal omission, or
Pendergast et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2016) 13:125 Page 2 of 15
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
meal frequency; (5) there was no correlate of meal skip-
ping reported; (6) the study design was one of the fol-
lowing; case reports, opinion articles, reviews, narrative
reviews, systematic reviews, meta-analyses.
Study selection
Two reviewers (FJP and KML) independently assessed
titles and abstracts for eligibility. Any articles that did
not meet eligibility criteria were excluded. Full texts of
the remaining articles were then obtained and screened
for inclusion. If consensus between reviewers was not
reached a third reviewer (SAM) was consulted and a
consensus approach was used.
Data extraction and synthesis
Data extraction was initially conducted by one reviewer
(FJP) using an electronic spreadsheet. Information ex-
tracted included author, year, country, design, sample
characteristics, participants’age, how meal skipping was
measured, definition of meal skipping; frequency of meal
skipping and correlates of meal skipping. Following the
initial data extraction the spreadsheet was verified for
accuracy and consistency by a second reviewer (KML)
with consensus reached with the help of the third
reviewer (SAM) in cases of disagreement.
Quality and risk of bias assessment
All included studies were assessed for quality and risk of
bias by two independent reviewers (FJP, KML) using the
Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics Quality Criteria
Checklist [30]. Any discrepancies between the reviewers
was resolved by discussion and consensus with a third
reviewer (SAM). Articles were assessed against 10 cri-
teria and were assigned a positive, negative or neutral
rating. As per guidelines, an article was deemed positive
if criteria 2, 3, 6, 7 and at least one additional criterion
was awarded a ‘yes’, neutral if criteria 2, 3, 6 and 7 did
not score a ‘yes’, or negative if more than six of the cri-
teria were awarded a ‘no’. Each criteria was assessed as
either ‘yes’or ‘no’. Criteria included: (1) the study clearly
stated the research question; (2) the selection of par-
ticipants was free from bias; (3) if study groups were
comparable; (4) participant withdrawal process was
documented; (5) the use of blinding was documented;
(6) participant compliance was measured; (7) the
measurements used were valid and reliable; (8) appro-
priate statistical analysis was used; (9) biases and limi-
tations were documented (10) funding or sponsorship
was reported.
Results
The study selection process, including reasons for ex-
cluding studies is summarised in Fig. 1. Of the 331 arti-
cles identified, 194 articles were screened based on their
title and abstract. Of these, 141 full-text articles were
assessed for eligibility and 35 studies were included in
the review. Study characteristics and risk of bias scores
are presented in Table 1 and Additional file 2 respect-
ively. Risk of bias assessment indicated four studies had
a positive rating (low risk of bias) [1, 31–33], 28 studies
had a negative rating (high risk of bias) [12, 34–60] and
three studies had a neutral rating [61–63].
Study characteristics
The included studies were conducted in 15 countries:
seven from the United States [1, 12, 31, 36, 44, 62, 63],
five from Japan [32, 33, 52, 53, 56] five from Turkey
[49, 51, 59–61] three from Nigeria [34, 50, 57] two
from Korea [37, 47], Ghana [35, 39] and Poland [54, 55]
and one from China [45], Croatia [38], Egypt [41], Greece
[43], India [40], Iraq [46], Saudi Arabia [42] and the
United Arab Emirates [48].
Of the 35 included studies, 17 consisted of primarily
female participants (>50%) [1, 12, 31, 35, 38–40, 43–45,
47, 50, 51, 58, 61–63], 10 included female participants
only [32, 37, 41, 42, 48, 52, 53, 55, 56, 59], six consisted
of primarily male participants (>50%) [33, 34, 36, 49, 57, 60].
One study had an even distribution of male and female par-
ticipants [54], and one study failed to report the sex distribu-
tion of its participants [46].
All but one of the included studies were cross-
sectional; the longitudinal study collected data at two
times points approximately 18 months apart [31]. The
included studies used a variety of different methods to
assess and define meal skipping. Dietary intake methods
were used by six studies; four used 24 h diet recalls
[12, 49, 53, 57], one used food records [44], and one
used a specially designed food frequency question-
naire (FFQ) [38]. This specially designed FFQ col-
lected information on the self-reported number of
meals and snacks consumed daily. Meal skipping was
defined as a non-reported meal within the respective
dietary assessment method. Binary response questions
were also used to measure meal skipping, with op-
tions “Yes/No”or “Regularly/Rarely”used by eight
studies [34, 35, 39, 41, 47, 50, 60, 62]. Question
wording influenced how meal skipping was defined in
these binary response questions. Meal skipping was
measured numerically by seven studies [1, 31, 32, 36,
46, 51, 54], and categorically by 14 studies [33, 37,
40, 42, 43, 45, 48, 52, 55, 56, 58, 59, 61, 63]. These
studies had varying response options and used varying
cut points to define meal skipping. Five of the 14
studies did not report how they defined meal skipping
from their response categories [43, 45, 48, 55, 61].
Meal skipping (any meal) was reported in 10 studies
[31, 34–36, 40, 45, 51, 54, 55, 62] and ranged from 4.8
to 83.3%, while the remaining 25 studies identified
Pendergast et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2016) 13:125 Page 3 of 15
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Table 1 Characteristics of the included studies
Reference,
year
Country Design Sample characteristics,
N (% women)
Participants; age,
years (mean ± SD)
How was meal
skipping measured?
Definition of meal
skipping
Frequency of meal
skipping
Correlates of meal
skipping
1. Afolabi
et al. 2013 [34]
Nigeria Cross-
sectional
University students;
140 (40% F)
N/R Q: Do you skip meals?
(Yes/No)
Q: Why do you skip
meals?
“Yes”; Text 53.6% M skipped meals,
35% F skipped meals
Reasons for skipping
meals:
-Time 48%,
-Appetite 19%,
-Cannot cook 13%,
-Illness 3%,
-Money 6%
2. Akarslan et al.
2008 [61]
Turkey Cross-
sectional
Young adults;
416 (59% F)
18-25 years;
(23.2 ± 0.97 years)
Q: Frequency of B, L, D?
(Almost always/Sometimes/
Very seldom/Never in a one
year period)
N/R 70% had regular
main meals.
Regular B 69.2%,
Regular L 75.5%,
Regular D 76.0%
SEX: (0 TMS)
3. Aryee et al.
2013 [35]
Ghana Cross-
sectional
Nurses; 220;
(66% F)
20–60 years;
(67.3% 20–30 years)
Q: Do you meal skip?
(Yes/No)
“Yes”53.6% skipped meals BMI: (+TMS)
4. Bahl et al.
2013 [36]
USA Cross-
sectional
College students
(Business);
353 (43% F)
N/R Q: How many days
during the last week
(0–7) did you skip
meals?
Numerical Participants who had
mindfulness training
skipped meals on 1.25
days during the last
week compared to
1.94 days for the
non-training group
MINDFULLNESS: (- TMS)
SATISFIED: (quality of
life – TMS)
5. Beerman et al.
1990 [62]
USA Cross-
sectional
College students
(Nutrition);
152 (56% F)
74% ≤21 years Q: Do you skip meals?
(Regularly/Rarely)
“Regularly”66% of on or off
campus students
skipped meals.
36% of those living
in Greek housing
skipped meals.
LIVING SITUATION:
(Greek housing -TMS)
SEX: (0 TMS)
Reasons for meal
skipping:
-Time 61%
6. Chung et al.
2003 [37]
Korea Cross-
sectional
College students;
180 (100% F)
20.41 ± 1.82 years Q: What is your breakfast
status?
(Rarely eating, Frequently
eating or Daily eating)
“Rarely eating”or
“Frequently eating”
74.4% skipped B BMI: (0 TMS)
7. ColićBarićet al.
2003 [38]
Croatia Cross-
sectional
University students;
2075 (53% F)
21.7 ± 2.0 years Specially designed FFQ Numerical;
Regular B, defined as
having B 6 or 7 times
per week
B consumed on 3.4
days/week,
L 6 days/week,
D 4.7 days/week.
32.2% F and 25.7%
M consumed B
regularly
SEX: (F + L, +D)
BMI: (+BS)
EXERCISE: (3.5 h versus
2.6 h of exercise per
week -BS)
Pendergast et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2016) 13:125 Page 4 of 15
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Table 1 Characteristics of the included studies (Continued)
8. Danquah et al.
2010 [39]
Ghana Cross-
sectional
University students;
150 (75% F)
64.6% 21–30 years Q: Do you eat breakfast?
(Yes/No)
“No”25% skipped B,
8% skipped L,
5% skipped D
ETHNIC: (Caucasian -BS)
SEX: (F + BS)
AGE: (15–20 years -BS)
COURSE TYPE: (Science
students + BS)
Reasons for skipping B:
-No time (57%),
-Not hungry (22%),
-Eat late at night (5%),
-Busy schedule (3%),
-No reason (13%).
Reasons for skipping L:
-No time (50%),
-Not hungry (25%),
-No reason (3%),
Reasons for skipping D:
-No time (38%),
-Busy schedule (12%),
-Watching my weight
(50%)
9. Deepika 2015 India Cross-
sectional
College students;
120 (80% F)
18–23 years Q: Do you A) Take all
three meals, B) Skip
meals with substitute
or C) Skip meals
without substitute?
“B”or “C”83.3% skipped meals Reason for skipping
meals:
-Time 40%,
-Taste 30%,
-Social desirable 28.3%,
– Habit 1.7%
10. Eittah
2014 [41]
Egypt Cross-
sectional
University students
(Nursing); 300
(100% F)
17–22 years;
(20.05 ± 1.62 years)
Q: Do you always
neglect to eat – B, L, D?
(Yes/No)
“Yes”72.7% skipped B,
7.3% skipped D,
6% skipped B and D
MENSTRUAL REGULARITY:
(Menstrual regularity -BS)
11. Eldisoky,
2003 [42]
Saudi
Arabia
Cross-
sectional
University students;
61 (100% F)
19–24 years Q: Do you usually
have B?
(Yes/Sometimes/No)
“Sometimes”or “No”63% skipped B,
61% skipped L,
31% skipped D
MOTHERS EDUCATION
LEVEL: (0 TMS)
Reasons for skipping meals:
-Hunger 48%,
-Time 31%,
-Weight control 21%
12. Evagelou et al.
2014 [43]
Greece Cross-
sectional
University students
(Nursing); 435
(83.4% F)
N/R Q: N/R
(Rarely/At least one/
Two/Three/Four/Five/
Six/Seven times a week)
N/R 31% skipped B SEX: (0 BS)
13. Freedman
2010 [63]
USA Cross-
sectional
College freshman;
756 (61% F)
N/R Q: Frequency of meal
intake? (Never/1-3
times a week/4-6
times a week/7 days
a week)
“Never”24.7% skipped B,
7% skipped D
LIVING SITUATION:
(On campus + BS)
SEX: (F + DS)
ETHINICITY: (Caucasian
-BS, -DS)
14. Huang et al.,
1994 [44]
USA Cross-
sectional
College students
(Nutrition); 1912
(68% F)
M 20 years, F
19 years
1 day-food record
(weekday)
Meal not reported
in food record
22% skipped B,
8% skipped L,
5% skipped D
SEX: (0 TMS)
Pendergast et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2016) 13:125 Page 5 of 15
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Table 1 Characteristics of the included studies (Continued)
15. Kapinos &
Yakusheva,
2011 [31]
USA Longitudinal University students;
388 (63% F)
18.1 years Q: Over the past year,
how many meals per
day did you typically eat?
Numerical 2.88 meals/day
at baseline,
2.61 meals/day
one year later
ENROLLING IN UNI:
(Second year of uni + TMS)
LIVING SITUATION: (M living
in dormitories with a dining
hall -TMS)
16. Kim et al.,
2010 [45]
China Cross-
sectional
First year University
students; 2427
(63.4% F)
18.9 years Q: Have you in the past
month skipped meals?
(Often/Occasionally/Never)
N/R Skipped meals at least
monthly in past year
-16.2%, Skipped meals in
the past week – 4.8%
INTERNET USE: (4 + hours/
day + TMS)
17. Lamia Dhia &
Ban Faud 2014 [46]
Iraq Cross-
sectional
University students;
350 (Sex NR)
N/R Q: No of meal/day?
Q: If missed what is the most
missed meal?
Numerical; Text 51.1% consumed < 3
meals/day.
Of those who missed a
meal 88.5% skipped B,
11.5% skipped D
Reasons for meal skipping:
-Time
18. Laska et al.
2010 [1]
USA Cross-
sectional
Young adults;
1687 (56% F)
18–23 years;
(20.5 years)
Q: How often do you
eat B, L, D during the
past week?
(Never/1–2 d/3–4 d/5–
6 d/Every day)
Numerical B consumption ranged
from 2.7 to 3.5 days/
week,
L 5.3–5.8,
D 6.1–6.5.
LIVING SITUATION: (Living
with parents + BS, +DS)
19. Lee & Yoon
2014 [47]
Korea Cross-
sectional
University students
(Food and Nutrition
50.3%); 159 (62.3% F)
18–28 years; 56%
18–20 years
Q: Missed meal?
Q: Reason of skipping
meal?
Text; Text 83.6% skipped B,
6.9% skipped L,
8.2% skipped D
AGE: (18–20 years + TMS)
Reason for skipping B:
-Time 61%,
-Habit 17.6%,
-Appetite 11.9%
20. Musaiger &
Radwan 1995 [48]
United
Arab
Emirates
Cross-
sectional
University students;
215 (100% F)
18–30 years;
(19.7 ± 1.3 years)
Specially designed
questionnaire
on meal pattern
N/R 15.8% skipped B,
11.2% skipped L,
7% skipped D
BMI: (0 TMS)
21. Neslisah &
Emine 2011 [49]
Turkey Cross-
sectional
University students;
400 (42% F)
19–24 years;
(21.7 ± 1.8 years)
1 × 24 h diet record Meal not reported in
food record
47.7% skipped B,
25.2% skipped L
SEX: (M+ BS) (F + LS)
22. Nicklas et al.
1998 [12]
USA Cross-
sectional
Young adults;
504 (58% F)
19–28 years;
(23 years)
1 × 24 h diet recall B had to equal or
exceed macronutrient
value of 1 serving of
milk.
37% skipped B ETHNICITY: (0 BS)
SEX: (0 BS)
23. Nzeagwu &
Akagu 2011 [50]
Nigeria Cross-
sectional
University students;
342 (63% F)
16–25 years; 81%
20–25 years
Q: What meal do you
usually skip?
Q: Why do you skip
meals?
Text; Text 27.8% skipped B,
16.9% skipped L,
5.6% skipped D,
4.4% skipped B and L,
2.3% skipped B and D,
.5% skipped L and D
Reasons for skipping
meals:
-Time 40.5%,
-Fasting/religion 6.7%,
-Weight control 10.4%,
-Money 9.9%
24. Ozilgen
2011 [51]
Turkey Cross-
sectional
University students;
408 (56% F)
18–24 years Q: How many times
a day do you eat B, L,
and D?
Numerical ~80% F skipped meals,
~72% M skipped meals,
~77% M skipped B,
~61% F skipped B
SEX: (M + BS)
Pendergast et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2016) 13:125 Page 6 of 15
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Table 1 Characteristics of the included studies (Continued)
25. Sakamaki
et al. 2005 [52]
Japan
and
Korea
Cross-
sectional
University students
(141 Korean), 124
Japan); 265 (100% F)
(20 ± 1.8 years) Q: Do you always take
breakfast?
(Daily/3-4 times per week/
Once or twice per week/
Rarely)
Everything except
“Daily”
21% Japanese
skipped B,
64% Korean skipped B
ETHNICITY: (Japanese –BS)
26. Sato-Mito
et al. 2011 [32]
Japan Cross-
sectional
University students
living at home
(Dietetics); 3304
(100% F)
18–20 years;
(18.1 ± 0.3 years)
Q: During the previous
month
how many meals have
you skipped?
Numerical B skipped 1.00 ± 1.74
times/week, L skipped
0.20 ± 0.73 times/week,
D skipped 0.32 ± 1.09
times/week
SLEEP: (Feel asleep later in
the night + TMS)
27. Shimbo
et al. 2004 [53]
Japan Cross-
sectional
University Students;
71 (100% F)
19–23 years; 1 × 24 h food duplicate
portion samples
Meal not reported in
food record
14% skipped B LIVING SITUATION: (Living
away from home + BS)
28. Suliburska
et al. 2012 [54]
Poland Cross-
sectional
Young adults; 600
(50% F)
18 years Q: How many meals
do you consumed
in a typical day of
the week?
Numerical 5% of overweight
ate >2 meals/day,
9% of normal
weight ate
>2 meals/day
BMI: (Overweight –TMS)
SEX: (F-TMS)
LIVING SITUATION:
(Rural + TMS)
29. Suliga et al.
2012 [55]
Poland Cross-
sectional
University students;
925 (100% F)
N/R Q: Frequency of main
meals (B, L, D, and S)?
(Daily/2-6 times a week/
Rarely)
“Rarely”11% rarely ate B,
8.2% rarely ate L,
12.6% rarely ate D
BODY WEIGHT SELF
PERCEPTION: (+ TMS)
30. Tanaka et al.
2008 [33]
Japan Cross-
sectional
University students
(Medicine); 127
(30.4% F)
(20.5 ± 0.8 years) Q: B consumption
(Every day/Not every
day/Completely skipping
everyday)
“Completely skipping
everyday”
15.7% skipped B FATIGUE: (+BS)
31. Tominaga et al.
2012 [56]
Japan,
Korea
and
Austria
Cross-
cultural
University students;
276 Japan, 103 Korea,
127 Austria, (100% F)
Japan; (19.9 ±
1.2 years), Korea
(21.5 ± 1.8 years),
Austria
(22.3 ± 5.2 years)
Q: Frequency of B, L, D
(Never/Occasionally/
Sometimes/Almost
every day)
“Never”or
“Occasionally”or
“Sometimes”
JAPAN – 50%
skipped B,
15% skipped L,
17% skipped D.
KOREA – 54%
skipped B,
51% skipped L,
46% skipped D.
AUSTRIA – 42%
skipped B,
29% skipped L,
47% skipped D
ETHNICITY: (Austrian –BS),
(Japanese –LD and DS)
32. Ukegbu et al.
2015 [57]
Nigeria Cross-
sectional
University students;
200 (47% F)
16–25 years 2 × 24 h recalls.
Consecutive days
including a weekend
day.
Meal not reported in
food record
41.5% skipped B,
21.5% skipped L,
7% skipped D
Reason for meal skipping:
-Time 42.5%,
-Weight control 23.5%,
-Fasting/religion 21.5%,
-Money 12.5%
33. Yahia et al.
2008 [58]
Lebanon Cross-
sectional
University students;
220 (56.4% F)
(20 ± 1.9 years) Q: Do you take breakfast?
(Daily/3-4 times per week/
1-2 per week/Rarely)
“Rarely”67% skipped B SEX: (M + BS)
Pendergast et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2016) 13:125 Page 7 of 15
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Table 1 Characteristics of the included studies (Continued)
34. Yildiza et al.
2011 [59]
Turkey Cross-
sectional
University students
(Medical); 301
(100% F)
18–25 years;
(21.2 ± 1.7 years)
Q: Frequency of B, L, D?
(Never/Occasionally/Most of
the days/Everyday)
“Never”or
“Occasionally”or “Most
of the days”
74% skipped B,
57% skipped L,
37% skipped D,
69.7% were
skipping at
least one meal
per day.
Reason for meal skipping:
-Time 46.7%
35. Yilmaz et al.
2014 [60]
Turkey Cross-
sectional
University students
(Medical); 995 (48% F)
M (21.25 ± 1.97
years); F
(20.94 ± 1.77)
Q: I usually skip meals?
(Yes/No)
“Yes”35.8% skip B,
27.9% usually
skip meals
DEPRESSION: (+BS)
N/R not reported, Mmales, Ffemales, Bbreakfast, Llunch, Ddinner, BMI body mass index, FFQ food frequency questionnaire, BS Breakfast skipping, LS Lunch skipping, DS Dinner skipping, TMS Total meal skipping,
(0) = No association, (+) = Positive association, (-) = Negative association
Pendergast et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2016) 13:125 Page 8 of 15
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
specific meals and their skipping rates. All 25 of these
studies reported breakfast skipping with prevalence rates
of ranging from 14 to 88.5%. Lunch skipping was re-
ported by 11 studies [39, 42, 44, 47–50, 56, 57, 59, 61]
with rates ranging from 8 to 57%. Dinner or supper skip-
ping was reported in 13 studies, [39, 41, 42, 44, 46–48,
50, 56, 57, 59, 61, 63] with rates ranging from 5 to 47%.
The majority of studies (28 of the 35 studies) exam-
ined correlates by examining associations between fac-
tors and meal skipping behaviours through Chi-square,
One-way ANOVA, Duncan’s’multiple range test and
regression (linear and logistic) statistical analysis [1, 12,
31–33, 36–39, 41–45, 47–49, 51–56, 58, 60–63].
Another approach used to examine correlates of meal
skipping (used in 10 studies), was the use of a ranking
methodology where participants were asked to rank po-
tential correlates against other meal skipping correlates
[34, 39, 40, 42, 46, 47, 50, 57, 59, 62]. From these ten
studies, ten ranked correlates were reported.
Individual influences (Intrapersonal)
Of the 35 studies included in this review, 33 studies
assessed correlates from the SEF that could be consid-
ered intrapersonal correlates. These included sex, age,
ethnicity, body mass index (BMI), education, menstrual
regularity, physical activity, internet use, and a list of
cognitive influences.
Sex
Sex was reported as a correlate of meal skipping by 12 stud-
ies; three reported meal skipping (any meal) [44, 54, 61]
and nine reported on specific meal skipping [12, 38, 39, 43,
44, 49, 51, 58, 63]. Two studies identified no difference in
meal skipping (any meal) in relation to sex [44, 61], while
one study reported meal skipping (any meal) to be more
likely in males [54]. Two studies reported no significant dif-
ference in breakfast skipping between sexes [12, 43], three
reported breakfast skipping to be more likely in males
[49, 51, 58], while two reported breakfast skipping to
be more likely in females [39, 44]. However, Huang
et al. [44] reported that females were more likely to
skip breakfast in summer months, this associations
was not present in winter months. Two studies
reported lunch skipping [38, 49], and two dinner
skipping [38, 63], both studies found females to be
more likely to skip these meals (lunch [38, 49] and
dinner [38, 63]) compared to males.
Age
Two studies reported an association between age and
breakfast skipping [39, 47]. Danquah et al. [39], reported
breakfast skipping to be more likely in those aged 15–20
years when compared to those aged 21–30 years. While,
Lee and Yoon [47] reported meal skipping (any meal) to
be more likely in those aged 18–20 years compared to
those aged 24–28 years.
Ethnicity
Ethnicity was reported to be associated with breakfast
skipping in five studies [12, 39, 52, 56, 63]. Of the stud-
ies that included Caucasian participants [12, 39, 56, 63],
three found breakfast skipping to be more likely in those
Fig. 1 Flow chart summary of articles identified in search and included in review
Pendergast et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2016) 13:125 Page 9 of 15
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
who were Caucasian compared with other ethnicities
(Japanese, Korean, African American) [39, 56, 63], and
one found no association [12]. Another study found
breakfast skipping and meal skipping (any meal) to be
more likely in Korean young adults compared with
Japanese young adults [52]. While, lunch and dinner
consumption was found to be more common in
Japanese young adults compared to Caucasian and
Korean young adults [56].
Body mass index (BMI)
Five studies reported that BMI was associated with meal
skipping [35, 37, 38, 48, 54]. Meal skipping (any meal)
was reported in four studies, two found no association
between BMI and meal skipping (any meal) [37, 48], one
reported meal skipping (any meal) to be more likely in
those with an increased BMI [35], while Suliburska [54]
found meal skipping (any meal) less likely in those with
an increased BMI. Breakfast skipping was reported by
one study and was more likely with those with an
increased BMI [38].
Education
Three studies examined education and its association
with meal skipping behaviours. Eldisoky [42], reported
maternal education status and its relationship with
breakfast skipping, although this was not significant.
Kapinos & Yakusheva reported those in second year uni-
versity were more likely to report meal skipping (any
meal) compared to those in first year university [31].
While, Danquah et al. [39], reported those in science
courses were more likely to report breakfast skipping
compared to students enrolled in humanities courses.
Menstrual regularity
Eittah [41] found breakfast skipping to be more likely in
those with an irregular menstrual cycle compared to
those with a regular menstrual cycle.
Physical activity
ColićBarićet al.[38], found breakfast consumption (6 or
7 times per week) was more likely in those who spent ≥
3.5 h exercising per week when compared to those who
did 2.6 h per week.
Internet use
One article reported meal skipping (any meal) to be
more common in those who used the internet heavily
(>4 h/day) [45].
Cognitive influences
Fatigue Two studies examined the association between
fatigue and meal skipping [32, 33]. Tanaka et al. [33]
found breakfast skipping to be more likely in those
experiencing fatigue, while Sato-Mito et al. [32] found
meal skipping (any meal) to be more likely in those
who’s mid-point in sleep was later (falling asleep after
1.30 AM and the mid-point of sleep falling at 5.31 ±
0.55 AM).
Psychological wellbeing Three studies documented as-
sociations between psychological factors and meal skip-
ping (any meal). Yilmaz et al. [60] found meal skipping
(any meal) to be more likely in those with depressive
symptoms; Suliga et al. [55], found meal skipping (any
meal) to be more likely in those with a self-perception of
being overweight; Bahl et al. [36] found meal skipping
(any meal) to be less likely in those who were mindful,
and meal skipping (any meal) to be less likely in those
who had increased body satisfaction.
Time Time or the lack of time was mentioned in 10
studies and when considered against other correlates,
time was ranked as the strongest perceived correlate of
meal skipping in nine of the 10 studies [34, 40, 42, 46,
47, 50, 57, 59, 62].
Hunger A lack of hunger was reported in four studies
and ranged in importance from being the strongest cor-
relate of meal skipping to the 3
rd
strongest perceived
correlate [34, 39, 42, 47].
Weight control Weight control was discussed in four
studies and ranged from being the strongest per-
ceived correlate of dinner consumption to the 3
rd
strongest perceived correlate of meal skipping (any
meal) [39, 42, 50, 57].
Money Money or the lack of money, was reported in
three studies and was ranked as either 3
rd
or 4
th
strongest perceived correlate of meal skipping (any
meal) [34, 50, 57].
Habit Dietary habit was reported in two studies, one
study ranked habit as the 2
nd
strongest perceived correl-
ate of breakfast skipping and the other ranked it as the
4
th
strongest perceived correlate of meal skipping (any
meal) [40, 47].
Religion Fasting/religion was reported in two studies,
where it was ranked as being either the 3
rd
or 4
th
strongest perceived correlate of meal skipping (any
meal) [50, 57].
Taste Taste was reported as being a correlate of meal
skipping (any meal), with one study ranking it as its 2
nd
strongest perceived correlate [40].
Pendergast et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2016) 13:125 Page 10 of 15
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Cooking skills Lack of cooking skills was reported by
one study as 3
rd
strongest perceived correlate of meal
skipping (any meal) [34].
Social environmental influences (Interpersonal)
Of the 35 studies included in this review, only one study
assessed a correlate that could be considered to be part
of the social environmental domain. The variable exam-
ined was the notion of “being sociable”. It appears to as-
sess participants’preference for prioritising social
activities over eating, and participants ranked it as the
3
rd
strongest perceived correlate of meal skipping (any
meal) [40].
Physical environment influences
Of the 35 studies included in this review, six studies
assessed correlates that could be considered physical
environmental correlates. These included rural/urban
living environments and housing type.
Rural/urban living environment
Meal skipping (any meal) was more likely in those who
resided in a rural area compared to those who lived in
an urban area [54].
Housing type
Five studies focused on specific living environments such
as housing types. Kapinos & Yakusheva [31], reported
meal skipping (any meal) to be more likely in those liv-
ing in university/college dormitories. Similarly, Beerman
et al. [62], reported meal skipping (any meal) to be more
likely in those residing with parents or in university
dormitories when compared to those living in Greek
university housing (fraternity or sorority housing). Indi-
vidual meal skipping events were reported in three arti-
cles [1, 53, 63]. Two articles reported breakfast skipping
to be more likely in those who lived away from home
[53, 63], while one found breakfast skipping to be more
likely in those living by one’s self or with parents com-
pared with living on campus [1]. This same article
reported the same association for dinner skipping [1].
Overview
In conclusion, majority of included studies (n= 33)
examined correlates found within the intra-personal
domain of the SEF, one study examined a perceived
correlate from the interpersonal domain, with six
studies examining correlates from the physical envir-
onment domain.
Discussion
Main findings
To our knowledge, this is the first systematic review to
investigate correlates of meal skipping in young adults.
This review identified that the prevalence of meal skip-
ping among young adults ranged between 5 and 83%.
The breakfast meal was the most frequently skipped
meal in comparison to the lunch or dinner meal, with
rates ranging from 14 to 88.5%. The perception of time
or lack of time was consistently reported as an import-
ant correlate of this behaviour, with nine of ten studies
rating time as the biggest correlate of meal skipping. Sex
was the most commonly reported associated correlate of
meal skipping: breakfast skipping was more common
among men and lunch or dinner skipping being more
common among women. However, the studies were dif-
ficult to compare because of inconsistencies in measure-
ment tools and definitions of meal skipping.
Breakfast skipping
This review identified that young adults skipped break-
fast more frequently than other main meals. These
results are consistent with studies of other age groups,
with the breakfast meal frequently reported as the most
commonly skipped main meal. A sample of American
elderly participants reported the prevalence of breakfast
skipping was highest (10.7%) when compared to lunch
skipping (8.6%) and dinner skipping (5.8%) [64], with
similar results seen in children and adolescent popula-
tions [65, 66]. Meal skipping was assessed in a sample of
college students with the breakfast meal never/rarely
consumed by nearly half of participants (44.2%), com-
pared with lunch (3.5%) and dinner (2.3%) [67]. This
highlights that different age groups experience meal
skipping at different rates and that different meals are
skipped at different proportions within each age bracket.
It is however important to note that within the literature
the breakfast meal is more frequently examined than
either the lunch or dinner meals.
Perception of time
The influence of time or the perceived lack of time was
reported within all ten of the studies that assessed
ranked correlates. Nine of the ten studies reported time
as the biggest perceived influence on meal skipping
when ranked against other important correlates of young
adult meal skipping. The young adult age period is char-
acterised by transition, including moving out of the fam-
ily home, commencement of further education and/or
starting a career [2]. These competing demands require
young adults to learn a range of skills, including priori-
tising tasks and coping with these new environments [3].
These findings are confirmed by previous literature, with
time scarcity being recognised as having a negative im-
pact on a range of eating behaviours [68]. Deliens et al.
[69] reported the impact of time scarcity on university
students, with results suggesting students preferred to
spend time on activities other than cooking, and
Pendergast et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2016) 13:125 Page 11 of 15
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
highlighted the importance of short meal preparation
times. Similar results are seen within adolescent popula-
tions, with 52% indicating that a lack of time in the
morning was the main reason for skipping breakfast
[70]. Therefore, perceived lack of time, may be a result
of varied prioritisation with healthy eating behaviours
poorly prioritised [69].
The notion of time is a temporal structure, self-
reported by individuals and can have multiple interpreta-
tions and ramifications [71]. Psychologists’such as
Zimbardo have defined an individual’s time perspective
as one of the most powerful influences on human behav-
iour [72]. However, none of the included articles pro-
vided a definition of time, which limited the ability of
the present review to identify how time may have influ-
enced meal skipping. For example, “time”may have been
attributed to food shopping, preparation, cooking time
or eating time and may have been interpreted differently
by individuals. Given that the perception of time is
underpinned by person-specific psychological constructs,
the methodological approaches in the included studies
were not detailed enough to provide a conclusive associ-
ation with meal skipping. As the examination of time as
a major barrier to health behaviours is complex, it
requires stronger definitions and measurement tools be-
fore it is able to provide comparable and valid results.
Further research is needed to examine, and develop an
understanding of trade-offs and prioritisation of lifestyle
factors seen between individuals.
Sex and meal skipping
This review identified that meal skipping (any meal) and
breakfast skipping were more likely in males, while
lunch and dinner skipping were more likely in females.
These associations are unlike those seen in other age
groups, with adolescent studies reporting breakfast
[7, 25, 66, 70] and lunch [66] skipping more likely in
females. The results seen within this review however,
are consistent with other research in this age group,
with results in undergraduates student samples (not
eligible for this review) finding no association be-
tween sex and breakfast skipping [73], and females
more likely to skip dinner [74].
Results across included studies varied, with many stud-
ies samples dominated by a single sex. Direct compari-
sons between sexes become limited when sample sizes
are heavily skewed towards one particular sub group.
Previous literature looking at the difference between
sexes and eating behaviours reports significant differ-
ences in food choices between sexes; females generally
have higher intakes of fruit and vegetables, higher in-
takes of dietary fibre and lower intakes of fat [75].
Females are also however highly motivated by weight
control and are more likely to diet or restrain their
eating behaviour [75]. What is currently unknown how-
ever, is the driver behind the apparent differences in
meal skipping between sexes and certain meals during
young adulthood.
Strengths and limitations
The present review has several strengths. It is the first
attempt to bring together the literature on meal skipping
correlates and employed a rigorous search strategy
whilst adhering to the PRISMA protocol [27]. In
addition, the use the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics
Quality Criteria Checklist by two independent reviewers
to assess risk of bias [30], and the use of an established
framework for reporting eating behaviour correlates
[20], are regarded as strengths of this review.
An important limitation of this review is the lack of
consistency in the terminology and definition of meal
skipping and the measurement of these behaviours. Defi-
nitions of meal skipping varied; consuming three meals
on <2 days/week, failing to report a meal in a food diary,
to answering yes to “Do you skip meals?”This limitation
is paralleled in the study of breakfast consumption [76],
and meal patterns in general [77]. In addition, meal skip-
ping was captured via differing methodologies, including
food diaries, 24-h recalls, surveys and FFQ’s (which in-
cluded a specially designed item to assess the daily con-
sumption of meals and snacks). Each of these methods
has its own strengths and weaknesses and are aimed at
capturing dietary intake rather than the omission of eat-
ing occasions [78]. Questions designed to evaluate meal
skipping were not consistent between studies, with con-
tinuous scales, binary and categorical responses utilised.
These inconsistencies in definition and measurement
limited our ability to compare the findings between
studies. Furthermore, multiple methodological and
reporting weaknesses were apparent in the reviewed arti-
cles. This was confirmed in our bias risk assessment,
where 28 of the 35 studies scored a negative ranking.
Results of the studies were poorly reported, with limited
use of appropriate statistical analyses.
Another limitation of this review is the classification
of young adults by age. Some included studies included
participants outside of the 18–30 year age range, while
the studies that reported university or college student
samples failed to report the percentage of mature age
students. Results therefore may not always be reflective
of young adult populations.
Moreover, given that the majority of studies were
cross-sectional, studies were not able to infer causation
and thus the direction of the relationship for influences
such as menstrual regulation and BMI was not clear. In
addition, the generalisability of our findings may be lim-
ited by predominantly female populations and wide
country-specific variations in religion, eating culture and
Pendergast et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2016) 13:125 Page 12 of 15
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
socio-economic status. The subjectively ranked attri-
butes of meal skipping provided a valuable insight into
why young adults may skip meals. However, these results
should be interpreted with caution due to the lack of
clarity in the data collection methodology employed in
these studies. For example, studies did not report if
questions were open-ended or categorical, which may
have impacted the results. It was also unclear in many
cases if these questions were framed in terms of meal
skipping in general or if it was directed at a specific meal
(e.g. the breakfast meal), which is important given that
meal skipping correlates appear to vary between meals.
Implications for future research
This review highlighted several implications for future
research. Firstly, definitions used to identify meal skipping
are inconsistent. A standardised approach to defining meal
skipping would provide clarity and allow for more reprodu-
cible results across studies. Secondly, the measurement of
meal skipping in existing studies is inconsistent. With a high
number of methods used to quantify meal skipping identi-
fied by this review, there is a need to standardise measure-
ment so that more informative comparisons can be made.
Thirdly, many of the reported correlates were within the
intrapersonal domain of the SEF. This highlights the need
to assess associations between correlates outside this do-
main such as physical environmental influences, to further
examine why young adults are partaking in this unhealthy
eating behaviour. Fourthly, this review focused only on the
young adult population, future reviews should be conducted
to understand correlates of meal skipping in different popu-
lation groups e.g. elderly or child populations. Lastly, only
four of the 35 studies had a positive risk of bias assessment
score indicating that future nutrition research needs stron-
ger design and reporting strategies. The Strobe-NUT [79]
reporting guidelines are aimed at improving the reporting of
observational studies with a focus on diet and health and
should be employed in future research to increase transpar-
ency and consistency of nutritional epidemiology studies.
Conclusions
This systematic review addressed a gap in the literature on
the correlates of meal skipping in young adults. Results
are consistent with previous research reporting that the
breakfast meal is the most commonly skipped meal for
this age group. This review highlights the perceived lack
of time to be an important correlate of meal skipping. The
sex of an individual was also reported to be an important
correlate of meal skipping, with males more likely to skip
breakfast and females more likely to skip lunch or dinner.
Therefore, sex and meal specific components, and im-
provements in time management skills, may warrant fur-
ther investigation as effective strategies for interventions
targeting meal skipping in young adults.
Additional files
Additional file 1: PRISMA 2009 Checklist. (DOC 68 kb)
Additional file 2: Table S2. Risk of bias assessment of included articles
according to the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics Quality Criteria
Checklist [30]. (DOCX 57 kb)
Abbreviations
BMI: Body mass index; FFQ: Food frequency questionnaire; PRISMA: Preferred
Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis; SCT: Social Cognitive
Theory; SEF: Social-ecological framework
Acknowledgements
Not applicable.
Funding
FJP is supported by an Australian Postgraduate Award Stipend, KML is
supported by an Alfred Deakin Postdoctoral Fellowship, AW –None, SAM is
supported by an NHMRC Career Development Fellowship Level 2, ID1104636.
Availability of data and materials
Data sharing not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or
analysed during the current study.
Authors’contributions
FJP conducted searches, analysed results and wrote paper, KLM was the
second reviewer of abstracts and full text and helped with the writing of the
paper, AW provided conceptual and structural advice and support, SAM was
the third reviewer of included studies, provided conceptual advice and was
responsible for the final wording of the article. All authors read and approved
the final manuscript.
Authors’information
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Ethics approval was not required for systematic review. No identifying data
was obtained or analysed.
Received: 2 August 2016 Accepted: 21 November 2016
References
1. Laska MN, et al. Dietary patterns and home food availability during
emerging adulthood: Do they differ by living situation. Public Health Nutr.
2010;13(2):222–8.
2. Mulye TP, et al. Trends in adolescent and young adult health in the United
States. J Adolesc Health. 2009;45(1):8–24.
3. Betts NM, et al. Ways young adults view foods. J Nutr Educ. 1997;29(2):73–9.
4. Haberman S, Luffey D. Weighing in college students’ diet and exercise
behaviors. J Am Coll Health. 1998;46(4):189–91.
5. Australian Bureau of Statistics. Australian Health Survey: Nutrition First Results –
Foods and Nutrients, 2011-12. Canberra: Australian Government; 2014.
6. Piernas C, Popkin BM. Snacking increased among US adults between 1977
and 2006. J Nutr. 2010;140(2):325–32.
7. Australian bureau of Statistics. National Nutrition Survey: Selected Highlights,
Australia, 1995. Canberra: Australian Government; 1997.
8. McNaughton SA, et al. An index of diet and eating patterns is a valid measure
of diet quality in an Australian population. J Nutr. 2008;138(1):86–93.
9. Dubois L, et al. Breakfast skipping is associated with differences in meal
patterns, macronutrient intakes and overweight among pre-school children.
Public Health Nutr. 2009;12(01):19–28.
10. Timlin MT, Pereira MA. Breakfast frequency and quality in the etiology of
adult obesity and chronic diseases. Nutr Rev. 2007;65(6):268–81.
Pendergast et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2016) 13:125 Page 13 of 15
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
11. Deshmukh-Taskar PR, et al. The relationship of breakfast skipping and type
of breakfast consumption with nutrient intake and weight status in children
and adolescents: the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey
1999-2006. J Am Diet Assoc. 2010;110(6):869–78.
12. Nicklas TA, et al. Impact of breakfast consumption on nutritional adequacy
of the diets of young adults in Bogalusa, Louisiana: ethnic and gender
contrasts. J Am Diet Assoc. 1998;98(12):1432–8.
13. Kerver JM, et al. Meal and snack patterns are associated with dietary intake
of energy and nutrients in US adults. J Am Diet Assoc. 2006;106(1):46–53.
14. Ma Y, et al. Association between eating patterns and obesity in a free-living
US adult population. Am J Epidemiol. 2003;158(1):85–92.
15. Smith KJ, et al. Skipping breakfast: longitudinal associations with
cardiometabolic risk factors in the Childhood Determinants of Adult Health
Study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;92(6):1316–25.
16. Pereira MA, et al. Breakfast frequency and quality may affect glycemia and
appetite in adults and children. J Nutr. 2011;141(1):163–8.
17. Farshchi HR, Taylor MA, Macdonald IA. Deleterious effects of omitting
breakfast on insulin sensitivity and fasting lipid profiles in healthy lean
women. Am J Clin Nutr. 2005;81(2):388–96.
18. Silliman K, Rodas-Fortier K, Neyman M. A survey of dietary and exercise
habits and perceived barriers to following a healthy lifestyle in a college
population. Cal J Health Promot. 2004;18:281.
19. Sakamaki R, et al. Nutritional knowledge, food habits and health attitude of
Chinese university students–a cross sectional study–. Nutr J. 2005;4(1):1.
20. Story M, Neumark-Sztainer D, French S. Individual and environmental influences
on adolescent eating behaviors. J Am Diet Assoc. 2002;102(3):S40–51.
21. Sallis JF, Owen N, Fisher EB. Ecological models of health behavior. Health
behavior and health education: Theory, research, and practice. 2008;4:465–86.
22. Bandura A. Social Foundations of Thought and Action. Englewood Cliffs:
Prentice-Hall; 1986.
23. Robinson T. Applying the socio-ecological model to improving fruit and
vegetable intake among low-income African Americans. J Community Health.
2008;33(6):395–406.
24. SmithKJ,McNaughtonSA,ClelandVJ,CrawfordD,BallK.Health,behavioral,
cognitive, and social correlates of breakfast skipping among women living in
socioeconomically disadvantaged neighborhoods. J Nutr. 2013;143(11):1774–84.
25. Keski-Rahkonen A, et al. Breakfast skipping and health-compromising
behaviors in adolescents and adults. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2003;57(7):842–53.
26. Ene‐Obong H, Akosa U. Adolescents living in boarding houses in Nsukka,
Enugu State, Nigeria I: Meal patterns, nutrition knowledge and nutrient
intake. Ecol Food Nutr. 1993;30(3-4):179–93.
27. Moher D, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-
analyses: the PRISMA statement. Ann Intern Med. 2009;151(4):264–9.
28. Khan M, Lipke L. Snacking and its contribution to food and nutrient intake
of college students. J Am Diet Assoc. 1982;81(5):583.
29. de Castro JM. Macronutrient relationships with meal patterns and mood in the
spontaneous feeding behavior of humans. Physiol Behav. 1987;39(5):561–9.
30. Dietetics, A.o.N.a. Evidence Analysis Manual: Steps in the Academy Evidence
Analysis Process. 2012. Available from: https://www.andeal.org/vault/2440/
web/files/2012_Aug_EA_Manual.pdf. Cited 20 April 2016.
31. Kapinos KA, Yakusheva O. Environmental influences on young adult weight
gain: evidence from a natural experiment. J Adolesc Health. 2011;48(1):52–8.
32. Sato-Mito N, et al. The midpoint of sleep is associated with dietary intake and
dietary behavior among young Japanese women. Sleep Med. 2011;12(3):289–94.
33. Tanaka M, et al. Relationships between dietary habits and the prevalence of
fatigue in medical students. Nutrition. 2008;24(10):985–9. 5p.
34. Afolabi W, et al. Pattern of Fast Food Consumption and Contribution to
Nutrient intakes of Nigerian University Students. Int J Educ Res. 2013;1(5):1–10.
35. Aryee PA, et al. Prevalence and risk factors for overweight and obesity
among nurses in the tamale metropolis of Ghana. J Medical Biomedical Sci.
2013;2(4):13–23.
36. Bahl S, et al. Mindfulness: A long-term solution for mindless eating by
college students. J Publ Policy Market. 2013;32(2):173–84.
37. Chung H, Song M, Park M. A study of the anthropometric indices and
eating habits of female college students. J Community Nutr. 2003;5(1):21–8.
38. ColićBarićI, SatalićZ, LukesićZ. Nutritive value of meals, dietary habits and
nutritive status in Croatian university students according to gender. Int J
Food Sci Nutr. 2003;54(6):473–84.
39. Danquah AO, et al. A pilot study of the dietary and physical activity
behaviours of international students: implications for health promotion. Afr
J Food Sci. 2010;4(3):86–92.
40. Deepika D, Vinti D. Food preference and dietary intake of college students
in Haryana College. Rev Res J. 2015;4(8):ROR–1543.
41. Eittah HFA. Effect of breakfast skipping on young females’menstruation.
Health Sci J. 2014;8(4):469–84. 16p.
42. Eldisoky ST. Interactive relation between dietary habits and spread of
malnutrition diseases. Ann Agric Sci (Cairo). 2003;48(2):663–75.
43. Evagelou E, et al. Exploration of Nursing students’ dietary habits. Health Sci
J. 2014;8(4):452–68. 17p.
44. Huang YL, et al. What do college students eat? Food selection and meal
pattern. Nutr Res. 1994;14(8):1143–53.
45. Kim JH, et al. Brief report: Predictors of heavy Internet use and associations
with health-promoting and health risk behaviors among Hong Kong
university students. J Adolesc. 2010;33(1):215–20.
46. Lamia Dhia A, Ban Faud I. Knowledge and practice of dietary habits and
healthy lifestyle in a sample of medical and non medical college students
in Baghdad. Middle East J Fam Med. 2014;12(3):37–47.
47. Lee JE, Yoon WY. A study of dietary habits and eating-out behavior of
college students in Cheongju area. Technol Health Care. 2014;22(3):435–42.
48. Musaiger AO, Radwan HM. Social and dietary factors associated with obesity
in university female students in United Arab Emirates. J R Soc Health.
1995;115(2):96–9.
49. Neslişah R, Emine AY. Energy and nutrient intake and food patterns among
Turkish university students. Nutr Res Pract. 2011;5(2):117–23.
50. Nzeagwu OC, Akagu JC. Food habits and nutritional status of Nigerian
undergraduates. Food. 2011;5(Special Issue 1):89–95.
51. Ozilgen S. Gender is correlated with body mass index, eating habits and
exercise frequency in students attending a private university in western
Turkey. Acta Aliment (Budapest). 2011;40(3):400–6.
52. Sakamaki R, et al. A comparative study of food habits and body shape
perception of university students in Japan and Korea. Nutr J. 2005;4:31.
53. Shimbo S, et al. Effects of life away from home and physical exercise on
nutrient intake and blood/serum parameters among girl students in Japan.
Tohoku J Exp Med. 2004;203(4):275–86.
54. Suliburska J, et al. Analysis of lifestyle of young adults in the rural and urban
areas. Ann Agric Environ Med. 2012;19(1):135–9.
55. Suliga E, Wronka I, Pawlinska-Chmara R. Nutritional Habits of Female
University Students in Relation to Self-Perception of Body. Biomedical Hum
Kinet. 2012;4:98–102.
56. Tominaga M, et al. Differences in self-assessment regarding eating
behaviors among female university students living in Japan, Korea, and
Austria. Food Nutr Sci. 2012;3(12):1673–81.
57. Ukegbu PO, Uwaegbute AC, Usonka VA. Contribution of snacks to energy
and nutrient intake of undergraduates in a Nigerian university. Malaysian J
Nutr. 2015;21(1):15–23.
58. Yahia N, et al. Eating habits and obesity among Lebanese university
students. Nutr J. 2008;7:32.
59. Yildiza EA, et al. Evaluation of the dietary habits, body images and BMI of
Turkish University students who live in dormitory. Pakistan J Med Sci.
2011;27(1):85–9.
60. Yilmaz Y, et al. The relationship between medical education and eating
habits along with mental condition in medical students. Erciyes Tip Dergisi.
2014;36(2):75–81.
61. Akarslan ZZ, et al. Dietary habits and oral health related behaviors in
relation to DMFT indexes of a group of young adult patients attending a
dental school. Med Oral Patol Oral Cir Bucal. 2008;13(12):E800–7.
62. Beerman KA, Jennings G, Crawford S. The effect of student residence on
food choice. J Am Coll Health. 1990;38:215–20.
63. Freedman MR. Gender, residence and ethnicity affect freshman BMI and
dietary habits. Am J Health Behav. 2010;34(5):513–24.
64 Lee CJ, Templeton S, Wang C. Meal skipping patterns and nutrient intakes
of rural southern elderly. J Nutr Elder. 1996;15(2):1–14.
65 Gross SM, et al. Breakfast and lunch meal skipping patterns among fourth-
grade children from selected public schools in urban, suburban, and rural
Maryland. J Am Diet Assoc. 2004;104(3):420–3.
66 Savige G, et al. Snacking behaviours of adolescents and their association
with skipping meals. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2007;4(1):36.
67 DeBate RD, Topping M, Sargent RG. Racial and gender differences in weight
status and dietary practices among college students. Adolescence.
2001;36(144):819.
68 Jabs J, Devine CM. Time scarcity and food choices: an overview. Appetite.
2006;47(2):196–204.
Pendergast et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2016) 13:125 Page 14 of 15
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
69 Deliens T, et al. Determinants of eating behaviour in university students: a
qualitative study using focus group discussions. BMC Public Health. 2014;14(1):53.
70 Shaw ME. Adolescent breakfast skipping: an Australian study. Adolescence.
1998;33(132):851–61.
71 Graham RJ. The role of perception of time in consumer research. J Consum
Res. 1981;7(4):335–42.
72 Boniwell I, Zimbardo PG. Time to find the right balance. Psychologist. 2003;
16:129–31.
73 Moy FM, Johari S, Ismail Y, Mahad R, Tie FH, Wan Ismail WMA. Breakfast
skipping and its associated factors among undergraduates in a public
university in Kuala Lumpur. Mal J Nutr. 2009;15(2):165–74.
74 Lee MS, Kwak CS. The comparison in daily intake of nutrients, quality of
diets and dietary habits between male and female college students in
Daejeon. Korean J Community Nutr. 2006;11(1):39–51.
75 Westenhoefer J. Age and gender dependent profile of food choice. Forum
Nutr. 2005;57:44–51.
76 Rampersaud GC, et al. Breakfast habits, nutritional status, body weight, and
academic performance in children and adolescents. J Am Diet Assoc.
2005;105(5):743–60.
77 Leech RM, et al. Understanding meal patterns: definitions, methodology and
impact on nutrient intake and diet quality. Nutr Res Rev. 2015;28(01):1–21.
78 Block G. A review of validations of dietary assessment methods. Am J
Epidemiol. 1982;115(4):492–505.
79 Lachat C, et al. Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in
Epidemiology—Nutritional Epidemiology (STROBE-nut): An Extension of the
STROBE Statement. PLoS Med. 2016;13(6):e1002036.
• We accept pre-submission inquiries
• Our selector tool helps you to find the most relevant journal
• We provide round the clock customer support
• Convenient online submission
• Thorough peer review
• Inclusion in PubMed and all major indexing services
• Maximum visibility for your research
Submit your manuscript at
www.biomedcentral.com/submit
Submit your next manuscript to BioMed Central
and we will help you at every step:
Pendergast et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (2016) 13:125 Page 15 of 15
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com